Simple tasks donât test brainâs true complexity
Rice University News Jun 14, 2017
Rice, Baylor College of Medicine duo details agenda for modeling problem–solving networks.
The human brain naturally makes its best guess when making a decision, and studying those guesses can be very revealing about the brainÂs inner workings. But neuroscientists at Rice University and Baylor College of Medicine said a full understanding of the complexity of the human brain will require new research strategies that better simulate real–world conditions.
Xaq Pitkow and Dora Angelaki, both faculty members in BaylorÂs Department of Neuroscience and RiceÂs Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, said the brainÂs ability to perform Âapproximate probabilistic inference cannot be truly studied with simple tasks that are Âill–suited to expose the inferential computations that make the brain special.Â
A new article by the researchers suggests the brain uses nonlinear message–passing between connected, redundant populations of neurons that draw upon a probabilistic model of the world. That model, coarsely passed down via evolution and refined through learning, simplifies decision–making based on general concepts and its particular biases.
The article, which lays out a broad research agenda for neuroscience, is featured this month in a special edition of Neuron, a journal published by Cell Press. The edition presents ideas that first appeared as part of a workshop at the University of Copenhagen last September titled ÂHow Does the Brain Work?Â
The researchers said simple tests of brain processes, like those in which subjects choose between two options, provide only simple results. ÂBefore we had access to large amounts of data, neuroscience made huge strides from using simple tasks, and theyÂll remain very useful, Pitkow said. ÂBut for computations that we think are most important about the brain, there are things you just canÂt reveal with some of those tasks. Pitkow and Angelaki wrote that tasks should incorporate more diversity  like nuisance variables and uncertainty  to better simulate real–world conditions that the brain evolved to handle.They suggested that the brain infers solutions based on statistical crosstalk between redundant population codes. Population codes are responses by collections of neurons that are sensitive to certain inputs, like the shape or movement of an object. Pitkow and Angelaki think that to better understand the brain, it can be more useful to describe what these populations compute, rather than precisely how each individual neuron computes it. Pitkow said this means thinking Âat the representational level rather than the Âmechanistic level, as described by the influential vision scientist David Marr.
The research has implications for artificial intelligence, another interest of both researchers.
ÂThey can play the ancient game of Go and beat the best human player in the world, as done recently by DeepMindÂs AlphaGo about a decade before anybody expected. But AlphaGo doesnÂt know how to pick up the Go pieces. Even the best algorithms are extremely specialized. Their ability to generalize is often still pretty poor. Our brains have a much better model of the world; We can learn more from less data, Pitkow said.
Go to Original
The human brain naturally makes its best guess when making a decision, and studying those guesses can be very revealing about the brainÂs inner workings. But neuroscientists at Rice University and Baylor College of Medicine said a full understanding of the complexity of the human brain will require new research strategies that better simulate real–world conditions.
Xaq Pitkow and Dora Angelaki, both faculty members in BaylorÂs Department of Neuroscience and RiceÂs Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, said the brainÂs ability to perform Âapproximate probabilistic inference cannot be truly studied with simple tasks that are Âill–suited to expose the inferential computations that make the brain special.Â
A new article by the researchers suggests the brain uses nonlinear message–passing between connected, redundant populations of neurons that draw upon a probabilistic model of the world. That model, coarsely passed down via evolution and refined through learning, simplifies decision–making based on general concepts and its particular biases.
The article, which lays out a broad research agenda for neuroscience, is featured this month in a special edition of Neuron, a journal published by Cell Press. The edition presents ideas that first appeared as part of a workshop at the University of Copenhagen last September titled ÂHow Does the Brain Work?Â
The researchers said simple tests of brain processes, like those in which subjects choose between two options, provide only simple results. ÂBefore we had access to large amounts of data, neuroscience made huge strides from using simple tasks, and theyÂll remain very useful, Pitkow said. ÂBut for computations that we think are most important about the brain, there are things you just canÂt reveal with some of those tasks. Pitkow and Angelaki wrote that tasks should incorporate more diversity  like nuisance variables and uncertainty  to better simulate real–world conditions that the brain evolved to handle.They suggested that the brain infers solutions based on statistical crosstalk between redundant population codes. Population codes are responses by collections of neurons that are sensitive to certain inputs, like the shape or movement of an object. Pitkow and Angelaki think that to better understand the brain, it can be more useful to describe what these populations compute, rather than precisely how each individual neuron computes it. Pitkow said this means thinking Âat the representational level rather than the Âmechanistic level, as described by the influential vision scientist David Marr.
The research has implications for artificial intelligence, another interest of both researchers.
ÂThey can play the ancient game of Go and beat the best human player in the world, as done recently by DeepMindÂs AlphaGo about a decade before anybody expected. But AlphaGo doesnÂt know how to pick up the Go pieces. Even the best algorithms are extremely specialized. Their ability to generalize is often still pretty poor. Our brains have a much better model of the world; We can learn more from less data, Pitkow said.
Only Doctors with an M3 India account can read this article. Sign up for free or login with your existing account.
4 reasons why Doctors love M3 India
-
Exclusive Write-ups & Webinars by KOLs
-
Daily Quiz by specialty
-
Paid Market Research Surveys
-
Case discussions, News & Journals' summaries