Brain scans could help doctors predict adolescentsâ problem drug use before it starts
Stanford School of Medicine News Feb 25, 2017
Impulsive behavior in teens can go hand in hand with drug use, but the link is weak and doesnÂt necessarily predict future behavior.
Researchers reported February 21 in the journal Nature Communications that theyÂve found a way to improve predictions of behavior  using brain scans that can tell, in a manner of speaking, whoÂs bored by the promise of easy money, even when the kids themselves might not realize it.
That conclusion grew out of a collaboration between Brian Knutson, a professor of psychology at Stanford, and Christian Büchel, a professor of medicine at Universitätsklinikum Hamburg Eppendorf. With support from the Stanford Neurosciences InstituteÂs NeuroChoice program, which Knutson co–directs, the pair started sorting through an intriguing dataset covering, among other things, 144 European adolescents who scored high on a test of whatÂs called novelty seeking  roughly, the sorts of personality traits that might indicate a kid is at risk for drug or alcohol abuse.
Psychologists know that kids who score high on tests of novelty seeking are on average a bit more likely to abuse drugs. The question was, could there be a better test, one both more precise and more individualized, that could tell whether novelty seeking might turn into something more destructive.
Knutson and Büchel thought so, and they suspected that a brain–scanning test called the Monetary Incentive Delay Task, or MID, could be the answer. Knutson had developed the task early in his career as a way of targeting a part of the brain now known to play a role in mentally processing rewards like money or the high of a drug.
The task works like this. People lie down in an MRI brain scanner to play a simple video game for points, which they can eventually convert to money. More important than the details of the game, however, is this: At the start of each round, each player gets a cue about how many points he stands to win during the round. ItÂs at that point that players start to anticipate future rewards. For most people, that anticipation alone is enough to kick the brainÂs reward centers into gear.
This plays out differently  and a little puzzlingly  in adolescents who use drugs. Kids brains in general respond less when anticipating rewards, compared with adults brains. But that effect is even more pronounced when those kids use drugs, which suggests one of two things: Either drugs suppress brain activity, or the suppressed brain activity somehow leads youths to take drugs.
If itÂs the latter, then KnutsonÂs task could predict future drug use. But no one was sure, mainly because no one had measured brain activity in non–drug–using adolescents and compared it to eventual drug use.
No one, that is, except Büchel. As part of the IMAGEN consortium, he and colleagues in Europe had already collected data on around 1,000 14–year–olds as they went through KnutsonÂs MID task. They had also followed up with each of them two years later to find out if theyÂd become problem drug users  for example, if they smoked or drank on a daily basis or ever used harder drugs like heroin. Then, Knutson and Büchel focused their attention on 144 adolescents who hadnÂt developed drug problems by age 14 but had scored in the top 25 percent on a test of novelty seeking.
Analyzing that data, Knutson and Büchel found they could correctly predict whether youngsters would go on to abuse drugs about two–thirds of the time based on how their brains responded to anticipating rewards. This is a substantial improvement over behavioral and personality measures, which correctly distinguished future drug abusers from other novelty–seeking 14–year–olds about 55 percent of the time, only a little better than chance.
Go to Original
Researchers reported February 21 in the journal Nature Communications that theyÂve found a way to improve predictions of behavior  using brain scans that can tell, in a manner of speaking, whoÂs bored by the promise of easy money, even when the kids themselves might not realize it.
That conclusion grew out of a collaboration between Brian Knutson, a professor of psychology at Stanford, and Christian Büchel, a professor of medicine at Universitätsklinikum Hamburg Eppendorf. With support from the Stanford Neurosciences InstituteÂs NeuroChoice program, which Knutson co–directs, the pair started sorting through an intriguing dataset covering, among other things, 144 European adolescents who scored high on a test of whatÂs called novelty seeking  roughly, the sorts of personality traits that might indicate a kid is at risk for drug or alcohol abuse.
Psychologists know that kids who score high on tests of novelty seeking are on average a bit more likely to abuse drugs. The question was, could there be a better test, one both more precise and more individualized, that could tell whether novelty seeking might turn into something more destructive.
Knutson and Büchel thought so, and they suspected that a brain–scanning test called the Monetary Incentive Delay Task, or MID, could be the answer. Knutson had developed the task early in his career as a way of targeting a part of the brain now known to play a role in mentally processing rewards like money or the high of a drug.
The task works like this. People lie down in an MRI brain scanner to play a simple video game for points, which they can eventually convert to money. More important than the details of the game, however, is this: At the start of each round, each player gets a cue about how many points he stands to win during the round. ItÂs at that point that players start to anticipate future rewards. For most people, that anticipation alone is enough to kick the brainÂs reward centers into gear.
This plays out differently  and a little puzzlingly  in adolescents who use drugs. Kids brains in general respond less when anticipating rewards, compared with adults brains. But that effect is even more pronounced when those kids use drugs, which suggests one of two things: Either drugs suppress brain activity, or the suppressed brain activity somehow leads youths to take drugs.
If itÂs the latter, then KnutsonÂs task could predict future drug use. But no one was sure, mainly because no one had measured brain activity in non–drug–using adolescents and compared it to eventual drug use.
No one, that is, except Büchel. As part of the IMAGEN consortium, he and colleagues in Europe had already collected data on around 1,000 14–year–olds as they went through KnutsonÂs MID task. They had also followed up with each of them two years later to find out if theyÂd become problem drug users  for example, if they smoked or drank on a daily basis or ever used harder drugs like heroin. Then, Knutson and Büchel focused their attention on 144 adolescents who hadnÂt developed drug problems by age 14 but had scored in the top 25 percent on a test of novelty seeking.
Analyzing that data, Knutson and Büchel found they could correctly predict whether youngsters would go on to abuse drugs about two–thirds of the time based on how their brains responded to anticipating rewards. This is a substantial improvement over behavioral and personality measures, which correctly distinguished future drug abusers from other novelty–seeking 14–year–olds about 55 percent of the time, only a little better than chance.
Only Doctors with an M3 India account can read this article. Sign up for free or login with your existing account.
4 reasons why Doctors love M3 India
-
Exclusive Write-ups & Webinars by KOLs
-
Daily Quiz by specialty
-
Paid Market Research Surveys
-
Case discussions, News & Journals' summaries